BULLETIN 2000 FEEDBACK
Health inequalities and social inequalities in health
Paula BravemanI; Nancy KriegerII; John LynchIII
IProfessor of Family and Community Medicine, and Clinical Professor of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of California, San Francisco, Box 0900, 500 Parnassus Avenue, Room MU-306E, San Francisco, CA 94143-0900, USA
IIAssociate Professor of Health and Social Behavior, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
IIIAssistant Professor of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, and Faculty Associate, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, USA
In vol. 77, issue no. 7 of the Bulletin, published in July 1999, Murray, Gakidou, & Frenk (1) recommend assessing health inequalities by categorizing individuals only according to their health, without reference to other characteristics except perhaps geographical location. They repeat this recommendation in vol. 78, issue no. 1, published in January 2000 (2). In both articles, they contrast studying what they term health inequalities across individuals with studies of social inequalities in health, which examine the health of different population groups categorized a priori by their social position, typically reflected by measures of social class or material or social deprivation or well-being. In the first of these articles they advance the serious claims that studying social inequalities in health inherently prejudges causality, masks intra-group variation, and does not allow for scientific inquiry into other key determinants of health inequality (1).
It is unfortunate that Murray et al. felt a need to discredit research on social inequalities in health in order to recommend their approach; the two approaches are not only not mutually exclusive, but ask very different questions. We believe their argument is based on misconceptions, as illustrated by the recommendation to compare health inequalities across geographical regions but not social groups. Geographical comparisons are similar to social group comparisons in that both involve a priori selection of a categorizing variable based on knowledge indicating its likely relevance. Furthermore, differences between geographical areas are likely to reflect social characteristics to an important extent, and studying geographical variation in relation to socioeconomic and other social conditions can provide important insights into population distributions of health (36). By suggesting that their approach to health inequalities, rather than what they term the social group approach, ought to guide public health policy, Murray et al. create a situation in which their claims could be used (despite good intentions) to prevent social inequalities in health from occupying an important place on the global research and policy agenda. We also are concerned about the implications of the ethical perspective propounded in the article in vol. 77 for setting public health priorities.
Studying social inequalities in health does not mean prejudging causality or obscuring intra-group variation, any more than comparing health between geographical areas prejudges causality or prevents studying variations within the areas. Observing mortality gaps according to educational level obviously cannot be interpreted to mean that education per se is the cause; however, it points one in a promising direction by prompting questions such as: Why is educational level persistently associated with diverse health measures across time and diverse settings? How do people with different levels of educational attainment differ from each other in ways that could potentially explain the observed mortality disparities? And: Among people with low education, what might account for differential risks of mortality? In contrast, the univariate approach that Murray et al. propose, examining the distribution of health without considering other population characteristics, leaves one, after the initial descriptive step, only with the general question: What factors differ between individuals with better or worse health? This does not suggest even a general direction to look towards for an explanation of those inequalities. Comparing groups with each other imposes neither conceptual nor methodological limitations on examining variation within those groups; we do not understand why Murray et al. imply that these are mutually exclusive.
We also are unclear as to why Murray et al. deem the social group approach a hindrance to the scientific study of population health. The basis for using certain social factors as categorizing variables is the overwhelming evidence of their importance as health determinants, across diverse health outcomes, settings, and time periods (720). Relevant variables include measurable factors reflecting social position and material resources, such as income, wealth, education, or occupation; gender, race/ ethnicity, and other social categories are also likely to be relevant (21). We agree with Murray et al. that a lack of conceptual clarity in the use of terms describing social position, coupled with historical changes in work and distributions of occupations within the global economy, has made it difficult to compare groups over time and between countries; however, we see this as reason to support rigorous work to achieve greater clarity (22) not to abandon social description in favour of a technical solution that discards the key questions. Although knowledge about the specific causal mechanisms and most proximate causal factors explaining diverse associations between social position and health is lacking, it is critical to grasp that social position is often a crucial determinant of the more proximate causes and, as such, a key determinant of health. Available evidence amply documents that one cannot understand health and illness in individuals or populations without examining social position (715, 17, 23). Studying social inequalities in health does not prejudge causation. Rather, it ensures that the role of social factors is systematically and explicitly considered.
To be practical, public health science must help identify health determinants and ways to modify them to improve population health. That means selecting relevant variables (whose specification requires categorization) based on prior evidence, describing associations among variables, and determining which associations are causal. The issue is not whether or not to categorize social groups a priori in studying health inequalities, but whether a given categorization is informative in understanding and ultimately improving patterns of population health. One should always expect significant variation within a given social group, and some categorizations may be too crude to be helpful; but this applies to any categorization, not only social categorization. We contend that the risk of obscuring important information is greater when the appropriate a priori, i.e. prior evidence-based, social categorization is not performed than when it is. The solution proposed by Murray et al. is to array health across individuals without initial categorization: this delays categorization but does not avoid it.
As far as practicality is concerned, it should also be noted that the variables frequently used to categorize social groups can be useful in targeting policy, indicating identifiable population groups requiring particular attention because their underlying social characteristics (e.g. poverty) are likely to make it more difficult for them to be healthy. Furthermore, in focusing on social inequalities in health one is consciously focusing on disparities that are highly likely to be modifiable; even if all health inequalities were ultimately remediable (2), which is questionable, some are far more remediable than others.
Murray et al. also question the ethical basis for studying social inequalities in health, expressing concern that this approach entails caring more about ill-health among the poor than the rich (1). Individually and clinically, one certainly should be equally concerned about suffering, regardless of who is affected. However, at the population level, priority-setting in public resource allocation must consider the markedly unequal burden of ill-health and its major risk factors that are persistently borne by some groups, particularly when they have fewer resources or more obstacles to improving their health without assistance. The data amply document that, despite some exceptions, a disproportionate share of ill-health and premature mortality is borne by the socially disadvantaged. Similarly, it is well known that the needs of those with the least political and economic clout can more easily be overlooked or disregarded when reliable information is not available on social inequalities in health. Without information routinely and systematically describing the health of more and less socially advantaged groups, one cannot begin to ascertain the differential impact of policies on these groups.
Few countries routinely monitor social inequalities in health. New interest in this area has recently emerged partly because of concern over marked widening of disparities in wealth globally, and recognition of the strong links between wealth and health. In this global context, WHO, with support from the Swedish International Development Cooperation Agency (Sida) and extensive international input, launched an initiative in 1995 to help bring concerns about social inequalities in health higher up on the agendas of governmental and nongovernmental organizations, and to promote relevant policy-oriented research. One objective was to assist countries in developing capacity to routinely examine social inequalities in health, focusing first on poor/non-poor comparisons (24, 25). Despite disclaimers at the end of their July 1999 article, the criticisms made by Murray et al. leave the strong impression that they propose their approach to replace, not supplement, studies of social inequalities in health on the WHO agenda. That impression is reinforced by the recognition that, to our knowledge, this is the only public statement on social disparities in health that Murray et al. have made since taking up influential positions at WHO in July 1998, in a unit whose mandate includes producing evidence to guide global health policy as well as taking a lead in research on health inequalities.
WHO resources are needed to help countries face the technical challenges of studying social inequalities in health in ways that are useful for forming policies. Information on the extent of individual health inequalities in the abstract, without information on the social patterning of their distribution, may or may not make a different contribution but cannot replace information specifically quantifying and seeking to explain social inequalities in health. We therefore believe that the rejection by Murray et al. of research on social inequalities in health and their proposal to replace it at least on WHOs agenda with studies of health inequalities in the abstract, represents a setback, not an advance, in scientific inquiry directed at improving the publics health.
References
1. Murray C, Gakidou EE, Frenk J. Health inequalities and social group differences: what should we measure? Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 1999, 77: 537543.
2. Gakidou EE, Murray CJL, Frenk J. A framework for measuring health inequality. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 2000, 78: 4254.
3. Shaw M et al. The widening gap: health inequalities and policy in Britain. Bristol, Policy Press, University of Bristol, 1999.
4. Kaplan GA. People and places: contrasting perspectives on the association between social class and health. International Journal of Health Services, 1996, 26: 507519.
5. Macintyre S, MacIver S, Sooman A. Area, class, and health: should we be focusing on places or people? Journal of Social Policy, 1993, 22: 213234.
6. Davey Smith G et al. Individual social class, area-based deprivation, cardiovascular disease risk factors, and mortality: the Renfrew and Paisley study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health,1998, 52: 399405.
7. Berkman L, Kawachi I, eds. Social epidemiology. New York, Oxford University Press, in press.
8. Antonovsky A. Social class, life expectancy and overall mortality. The Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 1967, 45: 3173.
9. Amick BC et al., eds. Society and health. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995.
10. Adler NE et al. Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 1994, 49: 15 24.
11. Evans RG, Marer ML, Marmor TR. Why are some people healthy and others not? The determinants of health of populations. New York, Aldine de Gruyter, 1994.
12. Feinstein JS. The relationship between socioeconomic status and health: a review of the literature. The Millbank Quarterly, 1993, 71: 279322.
13. Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Shema SJ. Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. New England Journal of Medicine, 1997, 337:18891895.
14. Marmot MG, Kogevinas M, Elston MA. Social/economic status and disease . Annual Review of Public Health, 1987, 8: 111135.
15. Acheson D et al. Independent inquiry into inequalities in health. London, The Stationery Office, 1998.
16. Black D, Morris JN, Smith C, Townsend P. The Black Report. In: Townsend P, Davidson N, eds. Inequalities in health: the Black Report. London, Penguin, 1988.
17. Marmot MG. Social differentials in health within and between populations. In: Health and wealth, Daedalus. Journal of American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 1994, 123 (4): 197216.
18. Mackenbach JP et al. Socioeconomic inequalities in morbidity and mortality in western Europe. The EU working group on socioeconomic inequalities in health. Lancet, 1997, 349: 16551659.
19. Child mortality in developing countries: socioeconomic differentials, trends and implications. New York, United Nations, 1991.
20. Braveman P, Egerter S, Marchi K. The prevalence of low income among childbearing women in California: implications for the private and public sectors. American Journal of Public Health, 1999, 89: 868874.
21. Krieger N et al. Racism, sexism, and social class: implications for studies of health, disease, and well-being. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 1993, 9 (6 Suppl): 82122.
22. Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss NE. Measuring social class in U.S. public health research: concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annual Review of Public Health, 1997, 18: 341378.
23. Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT. Why do poor people behave poorly? Variations in adult health behaviour and psychosocial characteristics, by stage of the socioeconomic life-course. Social Science and Medicine,1997, 44: 809819.
24. Equity in health and health care: a WHO/SIDA initiative. Geneva, World Health Organization, 1996 (unpublished document WHO/ARA/96.1).
25. Braveman P. Monitoring equity in health: a policy-oriented approach in low- and middle-income countries. Geneva, World Health Organization 1998 (unpublished document WHO/CHS/HSS98.1).