Death rate variation in US subpopulations
David A. Kindig,1 Christopher L. Seplaki,2 & Donald L. Libby3
OBJECTIVE: To account for variations in death rates in population subgroups of the USA.
METHODS: Factors associated with age-adjusted death rates in 366 metropolitan and non- metropolitan areas of the United States were examined for 199092. The rates ranged from 690 to 1108 per 100 000 population (mean = 885 ± 78 per 100 000).
FINDINGS: Least squares regression analysis explained 71% of this variance. Factors with the strongest independent positive association were ethnicity (African-American), less than a high school education, high Medicare expenditures, and location in western or southern regions. Factors with the strongest independent negative associations were employment in agriculture and forestry, ethnicity (Hispanic) and per capita income.
CONCLUSION: Additional research at the individual level is needed to determine if these associations are causal, since some of the factors with the strongest associations, such as education, have long latency periods.
Keywords Mortality rate; Socioeconomic factors; Ethnic groups; Geography; Health services accessibility; Regression analysis; Analysis of variance; United States (source: MeSH, NLM).
Mots clés Taux de mortalité; Facteur socioéconomique; Groupes ethniques; Géographie; Accessibilité service santé; Analyse variance; Analyse régression; Etats-Unis (source: MeSH, INSERM).
Palabras clave Tasa de mortalidad; Factores socioeconómicos; Grupos étnicos; Geografía; Accesibilidad a los servicios de salud; Análisis de varianza; Análisis de regresión; Estados Unidos (fuente: DeCS, BIREME).
Introduction
It is well known that death rates differ by geographical location in the United States (13). For example, a recent study of US counties for 1990 found significant variation in death rates for males aged 6176.2 years and for females aged 7082.6 years (4). However, the reasons for such differences remain unclear. Current concepts in population health regard mortality to be the product of multiple determinants, such as medical care, genetics, the physical environment, the socioeconomic environment, and individual biology and behaviour (5). However, it is not known if almost any combination of determinants can produce optimal health, or if a smaller number of basic patterns dominate. These relationships need to be unravelled to guide financial incentives aimed at improving population health outcomes (6, 7).
To try to understand the basis of mortality patterns, we examined the geographical variation in age-adjusted death rates as a function of area characteristics and population composition (3, 4, 8, 9). Although this "ecological" analysis of aggregate population data cannot produce valid inferences about individual mortality risks, it can generate hypotheses that can be further tested with survival analysis at the individual level.
Methods
The study population consisted of 320 primary metropolitan statistical areas (MSA) and 46 areas that included non-metropolitan counties within state boundaries (four states had no non-metropolitan counties). Data were aggregated from the county level to the MSA level, using the definition of MSA boundaries that were in effect in 1996. All remaining non-metropolitan counties were pooled into one non-metropolitan area for each state. These geographical units are referred to as "metropolitan statistical areas or non-metropolitan balance of states" (MSA/NBS), and were chosen as units of analysis because of their intermediate size between states and counties.
The independent contribution of demographical and socioeconomic factors, as well as medical care access, to variation in age-adjusted death rates across the 366 MSA/NBS was estimated using linear regression analysis of data from the Bureau of Health Professions Area Resource File (10). The Area Resource File is composed mostly of summed county totals and population-weighted averages for adjacent counties within each of the multi-county areas. The dependent variable is the annual number of deaths per 100 000 population averaged over 1990, 1991 and 1992, and age-standardized by applying local age-specific death rates to the 1990 US population age distribution.
We chose to analyse differences in death rates using variables known to exhibit strong geographical gradients, including racial or ethnic identity (11), socioeconomic status (12), rural/urban differences (13, 14) and medical services (8). Specific independent variables were: census region; gender; racial/ethnic composition (such as Black and Hispanic, which are not mutually exclusive categories); socioeconomic composition (e.g. the proportion of adults aged 25 years or older who completed high school; per capita income; percentage unemployed; and the Gini-coefficient of inequality in household incomes in 1990 (15)); urban/rural composition (metropolitan/non-metropolitan dummy variable and percentage of labour force in agriculture, forestry, and fisheries); and medical service measures (physician-to-population ratio, Medicare payments per person over 65 years of age, and the number of hospital inpatient days per person for short-term general medical and surgical procedures). The dependent variable was age-standardized, but not the independent variables, which may produce biased regression coefficient estimates (16). An alternative method suggested by Rosenbaum and Rubin for controlling for age structure and reducing bias led to similar results and conclusions. Nevertheless, this reinforces the imprecise nature of estimates produced from aggregate data.
To reduce the effects of multi-colinearity among several regressors, we constructed a unidimensional scale from these highly correlated variables using principal components factor analysis (17). The unidimensional scale of socioeconomic disadvantage included percentage income, the Gini coefficient of household income, percentage unemployed, and percentage with fewer than 12 years of schooling (Chronbach's alpha factor = 0.76). Increasing values of the factor are associated with increasing levels of economic disadvantage. All 366 cases were used in the analysis, since outliers showed little influence, with one exception: in the equation that included physicians per capita, two cases showed extremely high levels of physicians and extremely low levels of mortality: the Iowa City, Iowa MSA and the Rochester, Minnesota MSA, both of which are small cities with large hospitals and specialty clinics. With these outliers excluded, there was no association between physician supply and mortality (see footnote to Table 2). This was the only major change in coefficients due to the influence of extreme outliers.
After estimating the regression equations, we used the final model to generate hypothetical predicted values of mortality, assuming associated characteristics were held equal to the national average at all places. Mortality values for each MSA/NBS were calculated as the sum of the regression residual, plus the predicted value of the regression equation with one or more regressors of interest fixed at the national average; remaining regressors could take the observed values at each place. Using the ArcView GIS system, these predicted values were then used to produce maps of hypothetical death rates with associated variables equalized across all areas.
Results
Table 1 shows the summary statistics for the variables in the analysis. The age-adjusted death rate had a mean value of 885 per 100 000 population, with a standard deviation of 78 and a range of 6901108. The results of the linear regression with all 14 independent variables ranked by the size of their positive and negative effects are shown in Table 2. The unstandardized coefficients indicate the average difference in age-adjusted death rates between MSA/NBS that differ by one unit of the independent variable. For more accurate comparison we also show standardized coefficients which indicate the average number of standard deviations difference in age-adjusted death rates between MSA/NBS that differ by one standard deviation of the independent variable. Typically, standardized coefficients less than 0.10 (in absolute value) are "trivial" or have non-significant t-statistics.
From the standardized coefficients, we see that the variables with the largest relative association with mortality are African-American ethnicity (0.44) and adults with less than high school education (0.29). From the unstandardized coefficients, a difference between geographical areas of one percentage point in Black population is associated with an average increase of 3.32 deaths per 100 000 population; and for adults with less than high school education, a difference of one percentage point is associated with an average increase of 3.17 deaths per 100 000 population, all else being equal.
Other factors that have independent positive associations (in order of standardized magnitude) are: Medicare payments per capita; location in the west and south regions (compared to the northeast region); hospital days per 1000 population; and non-metropolitan areas. Those with negative associations are: working in agriculture, forestry and fishing; Hispanic ethnicity; per capita income; the Gini measure of income inequality; and physicians per 100 000 population. Percentage unemployed had a positive association with the age-adjusted death rate, and percentage female a negative association, although these and other regional differences were small relative to their standard errors. This regression explained 71% of the variance across the 366 areas.Table 3 shows six models in which variables were added sequentially. Model 1 compares the north-east census region with the other three census regions, controlling for differences in population gender. MSA/NBS in which the proportion of females was one percentage point higher have an average of 10.13 more age-adjusted deaths per 100 000 population. On average, MSA/NBS in the south have 75.6 more age-adjusted deaths per 100 000 population than those in the north-east region. This model explains 31% of the variance across all areas. In models 2 and 3, the impact of racial and ethnic composition on regional differences in age-adjusted deaths is examined. Model 2 shows that places with one percentage point more African-Americans have an average of 4.39 more deaths per 100 000 population, and 51% of the variance is explained by this model. Model 3 includes the percentage of Hispanics in the area, and shows that areas with one percentage point more Hispanics have 0.66 fewer deaths per 100 000 population.
In Model 4, MSA/NBS with higher-than-average socioeconomic disadvantage scores also have higher-than-average mortalities, with an average difference of 18.79 deaths per 100 000 population for every standard deviation difference in the socioeconomic disadvantage factor. This model accounted for 60% of the variance.
In Model 5, the effect of the supply of physicians was examined. This component has a small negative effect on mortality, with 0.06 fewer deaths for each physician added per 100 000 population. The variance explained only increases by 1% to 0.61. However, when two extreme outliers were excluded, this effect changed to a non-significant positive 0.02.
Finally, in Model 6 independent differences in death rates between metropolitan areas (MSA) and non-metropolitan areas (NBS) were examined. On average, there were 8.83 fewer deaths per 100 000 population for non-metropolitan areas, but the effect is small relative to the standard error. The variance explained does not increase from Model 5 to Model 6.
Two maps were produced to illustrate these effects more clearly (Fig. 1 and Fig. 2). Fig. 1 shows the baseline distribution of age- and gender-adjusted death rates, with generally higher rates in the south and lower rates in the Midwest. Nevada has an unusually high mortality rate, as does West Virginia, even given the generally high mortality characteristic of the South. Texas, Oklahoma and Missouri have average death rates, in contrast to the deep south-east, and Florida has lower-than-average death rates. Fig. 2 shows the impact of assigning average levels of race/ethnicity and socioeconomic disadvantage, in addition to age and gender, for all areas. The most obvious effect is the improvement in the south-east, with the expected death rate in Tennessee, Kentucky and West Virginia falling to the average range, and that in the deeper south-east even falling below average.
Discussion
This analysis highlights the large, non-random geographical variation in age-adjusted death rates in the United States. Much attention devoted to the quality of the health care system necessarily focuses on process measures (18, 19), but large differences in such a fundamental outcome as mortality should stimulate a re-examination of measurement priorities to identify the causes of variation. Although the differences reported here focus only on the mortality component, if a health-related quality of life measure were added to mortality, to produce a summary measure of health outcomes, the variation would undoubtedly be greater (20, 21). For example, the five-year difference in male life expectancy across British social classes increased to a difference of nine quality-adjusted life years when the EuroQuol measure was combined with the life year component (22). It is therefore essential that data collection and analysis of health-related quality of life make information on variation available.
The utility and limitations of ecological analyses are described elsewhere (2325). The present analysis cannot approximate relationships at the individual level; instead relationships between aggregate characteristics are described. Consequently, the correlations described in this paper should not be used to predict effects of interventions at the individual level, without confirmation by individual-level data; and cross-sectional correlations should not be interpreted as causal relationships. The relevance of some ecological or community-level variables, such as income inequality, should not be ignored however, since these variables only (or partly) have a causal effect at an ecological level (26, 27). Indeed, it has recently been suggested that geographical small area analysis may hold the greatest promise for studying the variation in social group health differences (28).
Another limitation of this cross-sectional study is that it does not reflect the different latent periods that are responsible for some of the associations (29, 30); it is almost certain that the association between education and mortality has a very long latent period, and therefore data from earlier periods might display different (and perhaps more valid) relationships. In addition, using age-adjusted death rates masks the mortality at different stages of the lifespan, and can be dominated by mortality at higher ages. Subsequent studies should explore these associations at different life stages (31, 32). It is also possible to think of geographical populations as static, even though the demography is the result of births, deaths, and in and out migration; and analyses to control for the impact of migration on mortality are needed. A recent analysis (33) showed that male migration accounted for nearly all of the differences in death rates at the British district level. Finally, our decision to group together all non-metropolitan counties in a state could mask variation across these counties; future analysis might use some other geographical unit of analysis such as hospital service areas (34).
These issues notwithstanding, our analysis reveals associations that account for 71% of the variance in death rates. While this is considerable, almost a third of the variance is unexplained. A fuller model (e.g. 5) would include variables from the genetic, physical environment and individual behaviour domains. We are currently attempting to organize data, such as air pollution levels, rates of crime, obesity and smoking, for further analysis. In this study, the strongest association of age-adjusted death rate is with the percentage of the population that is African-American. While this is not a new finding (3537), the strength of this association is independent of income, education, physician supply and region, and deserves attention. Additional variables, such as smoking rates, may explain this association, although they may be closely correlated with other factors in this analysis. The chronic stress induced by racial tension (38, 39) might also be more important than commonly acknowledged. Also, measures that are subject to respondents' willingness to self-identify with broad social categories, such as African-American and Hispanic (40), may mask significant intraracial differences. However, given the nature of racial mixing in the United States and the renegotiation of ethnic identities, even these categories may lose meaning in the future (11).
The effects of education level and other socioeconomic variables are consistent with previous results on the socioeconomic gradient in mortality (4143), in contrast to the negative association for the Gini coefficient of income inequality (26, 44). We also expected to find rural/urban differences, but the favourable association of the percentage of labour force working in traditional rural occupations (agriculture, forestry, and fishing), and the opposite independent association from simply being in a non-metropolitan county is unexpected. The medical care variables are plagued with endogeneity issues and high mortality is likely to cause the high levels of Medicare expenditures and hospital days. The supply of physicians is less likely to be endogenous, but the results show a non-significant association after eliminating two outliers. A recent similar analysis of years of potential life lost demonstrated that more specialist physicians were associated with lower mortality, but that generalists were associated with increased mortality in metropolitan areas (45). Since we studied only the total number of physicians, the opposite effects of specialists and generalists may offset one another to yield no net effect.
Independent of all other regressors in our models, the south and west regions tend to have higher mortality, and the Midwest lower mortality, compared with the north-east region. However, these differences are sensitive to model specification and with successive variable additions, the regions are affected in different ways. The high mortality in the south, so readily apparent in a univariate map (Fig. 1), almost completely disappears when race and socioeconomic differences are controlled, while the lower mortality in the Midwest improves even more. This "Midwest advantage" requires further examination to determine what factors, perhaps genetic or behavioural (46), are responsible.
While ecological and cross-sectional analysis cannot identify causal relationships, it is likely that race and socioeconomic factors will have causal implications at the individual level, and thus have policy implications as well. Race per se is not subject to policy manipulation, although related social factors underlying it may be. Certainly, education has health and other social benefits and is one variable that is the subject of local and national policy. While the causal mechanism of education on health outcomes requires more research, the length of its latent period calls for policy attention if improvements in health outcomes are to be accomplished as quickly as possible. The lack of significance of some medical care variables does not mean they are not important in improving health outcomes (47): they have been important in achieving current health outcomes and will continue to be important. But more detailed analysis is needed to determine which aspects of medical care will contribute most to health outcomes in the coming decades.
Finally, the possibility of a comprehensive health production function, the "fantasy equation" (48), that relates multiple determinants of health to health outcomes, has been discussed. Some of the methodological difficulties that compromise this approach have already been mentioned, but this approach is more difficult when the factors of production are both public and private, and collaboration across sectors is difficult or impossible (49, 50). The amount of effort that has gone into understanding the health production function is low, given its importance for public and private policy and expenditure (51). Recent global and national efforts to increase support for a broader understanding are welcome, but more will be needed if the goal of optimizing and making more equitable the distribution of length and quality of life to all persons is to be realized in the new millennium.
Acknowledgements
We wish to acknowledge partial support from the Milbank Memorial Fund, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH Trainee Grant Program, Grant Number T32-MH18029) and the National Institute of Aging. We also express our appreciation to Judy Knutson for clerical assistance.
Conflicts of interest: none declared.
Résumé
Variation des taux de mortalité dans des sous-populations des Etats-Unis d'Amérique
OBJECTIF: Tenir compte des variations des taux de mortalité dans des sous-groupes de population des Etats-Unis d'Amérique.
MÉTHODES: Les facteurs associés aux taux de mortalité ajustés sur l'âge dans 336 zones métropolitaines et non métropolitaines des Etats-Unis d'Amérique ont été examinés pour la période 1990-1992. Ces taux allaient de 690 à 1108 pour 100 000 habitants (moyenne : 885 ± 78 pour 100 000).
RÉSULTATS: Une analyse de régression selon la méthode des moindres carrés a pu expliquer 71 % de cette variance. Les facteurs présentant l'association positive indépendante la plus forte étaient l'appartenance ethnique (afro-américains), un niveau d'études inférieur à l'enseignement secondaire, des dépenses de santé Medicare élevées, et la localisation géographique dans des régions de l'ouest ou du sud. Les facteurs présentant l'association négative indépendante la plus forte étaient l'emploi dans l'agriculture et la foresterie, l'appartenance ethnique (hispaniques) et le revenu par tête.
CONCLUSION: Des recherches complémentaires au niveau individuel sont nécessaires pour déterminer si ces associations sont causales, car certains des facteurs présentant les associations les plus fortes, comme le niveau d'études, ont une longue période de latence.
Resumen
Diferencias entre las tasas de mortalidad de subpoblaciones en los Estados Unidos
OBJETIVO: Dar cuenta de las diferentes tasas de mortalidad de subgrupos de población en los Estados Unidos.
MÉTODOS: Se examinaron los factores asociados a las tasas de mortalidad ajustadas por edades para 1990-1992 en 366 áreas metropolitanas y no metropolitanas de los Estados Unidos. Las tasas iban de 690 a 1108 defunciones por 100 000 habitantes (media = 885 ± 78 por 100 000).
RESULTADOS: El análisis de regresión por el método de los mínimos cuadrados explicó el 71% de esa diferencia. Los factores para los que se halló una mayor asociación positiva independiente fueron el origen afroamericano, la carencia de estudios secundarios, unos gastos elevados para Medicare, y la residencia en regiones del sur y el oeste del país. Los factores con una mayor asociación negativa independiente fueron el empleo en la agricultura o silvicultura, el origen hispanoamericano y los ingresos per cápita.
CONCLUSIÓN: Es preciso realizar nuevas investigaciones a nivel individual para determinar si esas asociaciones tienen carácter causal, pues algunos de los factores más fuertemente asociados, como la educación, tienen periodos de latencia largos.
References
1. Kitagawa EM, Hauser PM. Differential mortality in the United States: a study in socioeconomic epidemiology. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press; 1973. Vital and Health Statistics monographs.
2. Morrill R. Development, diversity, and research demographic variability in the United States. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 1993;83:406-33.
3. Health, United States 1998, with socioeconomic status and health chartbook. Hyattsville (MD): Dept. of Health and Human Services (US), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 1998.
4. Murray CJL, Michaud CM, McKenna M, Marks J. US patterns of mortality by county and race: 19651994. Cambridge (MA): Harvard Center for Population and Development Studies; 1998. US Burden of Disease and Injury Monograph Series.
5. Evans RG, Barer ML, Marmor TR, editors. Why are some people healthy and others not? The determinants of health of populations. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1994. p. 378.
6. Kindig DA. Purchasing population health: paying for results. Ann Arbor (MI): University of Michigan Press; 1997. p. 194.
7. Kindig DA. Purchasing population health: aligning financial incentives to improve health outcomes. Health Services Research 1998;33:223-42.
8. Wennberg JE. The Dartmouth atlas of health care. Chicago (IL): American Hospital Publishing Inc.; 1996.
9. Pickle LW. Atlas of United States mortality. Hyattsville (MD): Dept. of Health and Human Services (US), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 1996. 1 atlas. p. vii, 209. DHHS Publication No. (PHS) 97-1015.
10. Area resource file (ARF). Washington (DC): Dept. of Health and Human Services (US), Office of Research and Planning, Bureau of Health Professions, Health Resources and Services Administration; 1997.
11. Williams DR, Lavizzo-Mourey R, Warren RC. The concept of race and health status in America. Public Health Reports 1994;109:26-41.
12. Feinstein JS. The relationship between socioeconomic status and health: a review of the literature. Milbank Quarterly 1993;71:279-322.
13. Smith MH, Anderson RT, Bradham DD, Longino CF Jr. Rural and urban differences in mortality among Americans 55 years and older: analysis of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study. Journal of Rural Health 1995;11:274-85.
14. Monroe AC, Ricketts TC, Savitz LA. Cancer in rural versus urban populations: a review. Journal of Rural Health 1992;8:212-20.
15. Welniak E. Gini.exe. Washington (DC): US Dept. of Commerce, Bureau of the Census, Income Statistics Branch; 1988.
16. Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. Difficulties with regression analyses of age-adjusted rates. Biometrics 1984;40:437-43.
17. Stata Statistical Software: Release 5.0. College Station (TX); Stata Corporation: 1997.
18. Chassin MR, Galvin RW. The urgent need to improve health care quality. Institute of Medicine National Roundtable on Health Care Quality. JAMA 1998;280:1000-5.
19. Bodenheimer T. The American health care system the movement for improved quality in health care. New England Journal of Medicine 1999;340:488- 92.
20. Institute of Medicine. Summarizing population health: Directions for the development and application of population metrics. Washington (DC): Institute of Medicine National Academy Press; 1998.
21. Erickson P, Wilson R, Shannon I. Years of healthy life. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 1995. Statistical Notes from the National Center for Health Statistics, No. 7.
22. Williams A, Culyer AJ, Maynard A. Being reasonable about the economics of health: selected essays by Alan Williams. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar; 1997. pp. 333-9.
23. Robinson WS. Ecological correlation and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review 1950;15:351-7.
24. Schwartz S. The fallacy of the ecological fallacy: the potential misuse of a concept and the consequences. American Journal of Public Health 1994;84:819-24.
25. Sinsheimer P. The risks of economic modeling before establishing the casual inference. Risk Analysis 1991;11:187-8.
26. Wilkinson RG. Income distribution and life expectancy. British Medical Journal 1992;304:165-8.
27. Robert SA. Community-level socioeconomic status effects on adult health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 1998;39:18-37.
28. Murray CJL, Gakidou EE, Frenk J. Health inequalities and social group differences: what should we measure? Bulletin of the World Health Organization 1999;77:537-43.
29. Barker DJ, Osmond C. Inequalities in health in Britain: specific explanations in three Lancashire towns. British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Edition) 1987;294:749-52.
30. Hertzman C, Frank J, Evans R. Heterogeneities in health status. In: Evans RG, Barer ML, Marmor TR, editors. Why are some people healthy and others not? The determinants of health of populations. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1994.
31. Holland P, Berney L, Blane D, Davey Smith G, Gunnell DJ, Montgomery SM. Life course accumulation of disadvantage: childhood health and hazard exposure during adulthood. Social Science and Medicine 2000;50:1285-95.
32. Elder GH. Human lives in changing societies: life course and developmental insights. In: Cairns RB, Elder GH, Costello EJ, editors. Developmental science. Melbourne: Cambridge University Press; 1996.
33. Brimblecombe N, Dorling D, Shaw M. Migration and geographical inequalities in health in Britain. Social Science and Medicine 2000;50:861-78.
34. Makuc DM, Haglund B, Ingram DD, Kleinman JC, Feldman JJ. The use of health service areas for measuring provider availability. Journal of Rural Health 1991;7:347-56.
35. Lillie-Blanton M, Laveist T. Race/ethnicity, the social environment, and health. Social Science and Medicine 1996;43:83-91.
36. Martinez RM, Lillie-Blanton M. Why race and gender remain important in health services research. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 1996;12:316-8.
37. Warren RC, Hahn RA, Bristow L, Yu ES. The use of race and ethnicity in public health surveillance [editorial]. Public Health Reports 1994;109:4-6.
38. Feagin J. The continuing significance of race: anti-black discrimination in public places. American Social Review 1991;56:101-16.
39. Dressler WW. Social class, skin color, and arterial blood pressure in two societies. Ethnicity and Disease 1991;1:60-77.
40. Sorlie PD, Backlund E, Johnson NJ, Rogot E. Mortality by Hispanic status in the United States. JAMA 1993;270:2464-8.
41. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, et al. Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist 1994;49:15-24.
42. Marmot MG, Kogevinas M, Elston MA. Social/economic status and disease. Annual Review of Public Health 1987;8:111-35.
43. Pappas G, Queen S, Hadden W, Fisher G. The increasing disparity in mortality between socioeconomic groups in the United States, 1960 and 1986. New England Journal of Medicine 1993;329:103-9.
44. Kaplan GA, Pamuk ER, Lynch JW, Cohen RD, Balfour JL. Inequality in income and mortality in the United States: analysis of mortality and potential pathways [published erratum appears in BMJ 1996;312(7041):1253]. British Medical Journal 1996;312:999-1003.
45. Mansfield CJ, Wilson JL, Kobrinski EJ, Mitchell J. Premature mortality in the United States: the roles of geographic area, socioeconomic status, household type, and availability of medical care. American Journal of Public Health 1999;89:893-8.
46. Behavioral risk factor surveillance summary prevalence report. Atlanta (GA): Dept. of Health and Human Services (US), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 1996.
47. Bunker JP, Frazier HS, Mosteller F. Improving health: measuring effects of medical care. Milbank Quarterly 1994;72:225-58.
48. Stoddardt G. The challenge of producing health in modern economies. Toronto: Canadian Institute for Advanced Research, Program in Population Health; 1995.
49. Lasker RD. Medicine and public health: the power of collaboration. New York (NY): New York Academy of Medicine; 1997. p. 178.
50. Kindig DA. Managing boundaries. Purchasing population health: paying for results. Ann Arbor (MI): University of Michigan Press; 1997. p. 113-32.
51. Kindig DA. Beyond health services research. Health Services Research 1999;34:205-14.
1 Professor, University of Wisconsin, School of Medicine, Department of Population Health Sciences, Milbank Memorial Fund, 610 Walnut Street, Suite 760, Madison,WI 53705-2397, USA (email: dakindig@facstaff.wisc.edu). Correspondence should be addressed to this author.
2 PhD candidate, University of Wisconsin, School of Medicine, Department of Population Health Sciences, Program in Population Health, Wisconsin, Madison.
3 Scientist, University of Wisconsin, School of Medicine, Department of Population Health Sciences, Madison, Wisconsin.
Ref. No. 01-1138